Skip to main content

Impulsivity mediates the relationship between sleep quality and interpersonal functioning: a cross-sectional study in a sample of university students

Abstract

Background

Amid the current mental health pandemic, research continues to investigate potential contributors associated with increasing levels of negative mental health. Among such contributors is sleep, which is vital for physiological and psychological functioning with potential downstream behavioral consequences, including in relation to impulsivity and social functioning. Given the significant rates of disordered sleep behaviors reported in the literature, our study sought to investigate the relationship between sleep quality, impulsivity and interpersonal functioning among university students.

Methods

An anonymous online survey was administered to university students (Ages 18 + ; N = 526; 33% male, 67% female) addressing demographics, sleep quality, impulsivity, and interpersonal functioning.

Results

Our findings indicate a substantial proportion of students reporting poor sleep quality and impulsivity. Moreover, higher levels of impulsivity and lower interpersonal functioning were associated with poor sleep quality. Mediation analysis revealed a significant mediating role of attentional impulsivity in the relationship between sleep quality and interpersonal functioning.

Conclusions

Repeated reports of significant levels of impulsivity underlying numerous psychiatric disorders, its prevalence socially, and the fundamental issue that impulsivity reflects (i.e., lack of self-control/self-discipline/self-regulation), suggests a necessity to reorient therapeutic efforts towards the root of the problem. Thus, efforts should seek to maximize preventative behaviors that strengthen the individual (e.g., improving sleep-related behaviors), and contribute to the building of character/virtue, through self-discipline, perseverance and consistency, in order to reduce negative outcomes (e.g., impulsivity, dysfunctional interpersonal functioning).

Introduction

Research continues to indicate growing levels of negative mental health among university students (e.g., Beiter et al., 2015; Bruffaerts et al., 2018; Emmerton et al., 2024; Limone and Toto, 2022; Lipson et al., 2019; Torales et al., 2019; Twenge et al., 2010). The correlates to negative mental health are many, and scientific research continues to seek further understanding of such correlates in all contexts, including in the university student population, in order to provide potential solutions. Among the numerous correlates identified in the scientific literature, including in our previous work, sleep continues to be an important source of stress/concern (Almojali et al., 2017; Alotaibi et al., 2020; Beiter et al., 2015; Dietch et al., 2016). Furthermore, substantial proportions of college students in the United States and globally report poor sleep quality and/or an inadequate amount of sleep (Albqoor and Shaheen, 2021; Becker et al., 2018; Dinis and Braganca, 2018; Lund et al., 2010; Mbous et al., 2022; Ramon-Arbues et al., 2022).

Proper sleep quality and appropriate/healthy levels of sleep play a significant restorative role physiologically, immunologically, hormonally, neurologically, and psychologically (Asif et al., 2017; Benington and Heller, 1995; Dattilo et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2015; Scott et al., 2021). Thus, the consequences of disturbed or inadequate sleep can have broad pathological implications ranging from the cellular to the behavioral levels. Under such situations, at the neurobehavioral level, executive functioning, which regulates basic behaviors such as attention (Logue and Gould, 2014) and impulse control (Hayashi et al., 2017; Hayashi and Washio, 2020; Jones et al., 2021; Narvaez et al., 2012; Reynolds et al., 2019), is reduced.

Effective/appropriate executive functioning is necessary in the processes of error detection (Simoes-Franklin et al., 2010; van Gaal and Lamme, 2012), emotional regulation (Mohammed et al., 2022) and social cue processing (Dias et al., 2022). Therefore, when negatively impacted by factors such as sleep deprivation, error detection becomes dysregulated (Thomas et al., 2000; Tsai et al., 2005). Additionally, emotional dysregulation can result, potentially due to a dysfunctional metabolism impacting amygdalar regulation (Benington and Heller, 1995; Thomas et al., 2000; Wu et al., 2006; Yoo et al., 2007). Consequently, this can lead to an enhanced sensitivity to negative stimuli, decreased responsiveness to long-term positive events, reduced positive moods, amplified emotional sensitivity, and increased irritability (Dinges et al., 1997; Sanz-Arigita et al., 2021; Yoo et al., 2007; Young and Nusslock, 2016; Zohar et al., 2005). The potential for more serious negative behaviors, including but not limited to, suicidal thoughts and behaviors, is also present (e.g., Pigeon et al., 2012; Tsai et al., 2005; Vedaa et al., 2019; Vyazovskiy and Delogu, 2014; Wu et al., 2006). Such consequences/behaviors not only impact the individual experiencing them, but also the social surroundings of the individual, as their behaviors can influence the way in which they interact with others.

Additionally, as indicated above, executive functioning is important in impulse control. A deficiency in impulse control can lead/contribute to a wide range of negative, disinhibited and often risky behaviors. Such behaviors include, but are not limited to, excessive texting, texting while driving, drug abuse and/or inappropriate alcohol use and risky sexual behaviors and any consequences resulting from such behaviors (Hayashi et al., 2017; Hayashi and Washio, 2020; Jentsch et al., 2014; Jones et al., 2021; Killgore et al., 2006; Narvaez et al., 2012; Potenza and de Wit, 2010; Reynolds et al., 2019). Related, inappropriate impulse control can also lead to adverse/dysfunctional social behaviors evident in such behaviors as inappropriate peer influence, social anxiety, aggression and antisocial personality disorder (aan Het Rot et al., 2015; DeBono et al., 2011; Dixon et al., 2017; Krakowski, 2003; Roeser et al., 2018; Swann et al., 2010). In addition to the potential effects of emotional dysregulation discussed above, the presence of inappropriate social behaviors, including dysregulation in social cue processing (Gilboa-Schechtman and Shachar-Lavie, 2013; Ruz and Tudela, 2011), can also subsequently influence interpersonal functioning (defined as intimacy and empathy; DSM-5) and the formation and maintenance of relationships (Albers, 2012; Chiu et al., 2021; Crocker et al., 2017). Ultimately, healthy social relationships, and therefore appropriate interpersonal functioning, have been shown to influence the overall well-being of the individual (e.g., Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2010; Hefner and Eisenberg, 2009).

Therefore, given 1) the prevalence and plethora of negative outcomes associated with disordered sleep behaviors on aspects of executive functioning (such as error detection, emotional regulation and impulsivity), 2) the various relationships reported in the literature between sleep, executive functioning, emotional regulation and interpersonal relationships, and 3) the importance of quality/healthy relationships in the midst of the mental health “pandemic”, our study sought to investigate the relationship between sleep quality and interpersonal functioning and the potential mediating role of impulsivity in such a relationship, in a sample of university students.

Methods

This survey research was conducted in compliance with Federal Law pertaining to the protection of human research subjects. Prior to administration of the survey, Franciscan University of Steubenville Institutional Review Board (IRB) approval was obtained (#2022–4). Our study consisted of a convenience sample of university/college (undergraduate and graduate) students from Franciscan University of Steubenville, OH, United States. An anonymous survey was sent via the university student email address, to all students taking classes at Franciscan University, who were at least 18 years of age. Over the course of two weeks (April 19th – May 3rd, 2022), the survey was administered through the online survey engine SurveyMonkey®. Prior to being provided access to the survey, participants were directed to a consent form, which detailed the confidentiality and the nature of the study and explained that participation in the study implied consent to analyze and publish the overall results. Participants who did not provide consent were directed to the Disqualification Page. The projected time of completion of the survey was approximately 15 min. The final page of the survey included a link to enter an optional drawing for one (1) of four (4) VISA gift cards ($25 each). The participants were informed that there was no possibility of linking the drawing information to that of the survey and that their information would remain confidential.

Exclusion criteria

Any individual who (1) was younger than 18 years of age (n= 0), (2) was not a student at Franciscan University of Steubenville (n= 4), (3) was an online-only student (n= 185), or (4) responded “No” to the consent question (n = 3) was immediately directed to the Disqualification Page. Additionally, 75 individuals exited the survey prior to completing any or all of the above required criteria.

Exclusion criteria included any individual who: (1) did not complete the survey question regarding their age (n= 14), or (2) did not provide a response to (n = 46) or complete the required questions (n = 21) for the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). The final number of participants whose responses met inclusion criteria was 526 (89% of the 593 respondents who started the survey; 22% of the 2386 students in the target population who received the invitation email), which is representative of the target population.

Survey structure

Demographic questions

Demographic questions included: age, sex, primary major, and living status during the school year. Participants were also asked questions regarding the average number of hours a day they spend on academics outside of scheduled classes, number of credit hours currently being taken and the number of semesters they have spent living on campus at Franciscan University (including the semester currently underway), as well as on other university campuses (i.e., not Franciscan University).

Additionally, participants were asked to complete the “Daily Spiritual Experiences” domain of the Brief Multidimensional Measurement of Religiousness/Spirituality, which is comprised of a number of domains that address various aspects pertaining to religiosity and spirituality, which can be utilized separately (Fetzer Institute / National Institute on Aging Working Group, 2003). The “Daily Spiritual Experiences” domain consisted of 7 items measured on a six-point Likert scale (1 =Many times a day to 6 = Never or almost never).

Pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI)

The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) (Buysse et al., 1989) was used to assess participants’ sleep quality and disturbances over the past month. Subjects were asked questions regarding various aspects of their sleep habits, including the rating of statements on various Likert scales (e.g., 0 =Not during the past month to 3 = Three or more times a week). The PSQI is comprised of seven components (subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleeping medication, daytime dysfunction), which are “standardized versions of areas routinely assessed in clinical interviews of patients with sleep/wake complaints” (Buysse et al., 1989). The scores of the seven components are then summed to create a global/total PSQI score (ranging from 0–21), with higher scores being indicative of worse sleep quality. Moreover, a global PSQI score greater than 5 has been previously indicated as a reliable cutoff for indicating “poor” versus “good” sleepers (Buysse et al., 1989). Thus, going forward, in this study, in regards to our findings, sleep quality refers to the broad definition reflected in the PSQI consisting of the seven components addressed above, addressing both aspects of sleep quality and sleep disturbance.

The question regarding a roommate or bed partner originally included additional components scored on a four-point Likert scale. However, for the purpose of our survey, this question, which does not contribute to the global score, was modified to simply ask if the participant had a roommate and if so, whether they were in the same room or another room.

Barratt impulsiveness scale (BIS-11)

Our survey also included the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11 (BIS-11) (Patton et al., 1995) to assess various components of trait impulsivity. Participants were asked to indicate the appropriate response (on a four-point Likert scale:Rarely/Never to Almost always/Always) regarding various statements pertaining to ways in which they act and think. A total score (Total BIS) was calculated, as well as scores for each of the three subscales: Attentional Impulsiveness (AttImp), Motor Impulsiveness (MotImp), Non-planning Impulsiveness (NpImp), with higher scores reflecting higher impulsivity for both the subscales and overall scores. Cronbach’s alpha indicated good internal consistency for Total BIS (α = 0.84), and acceptable for the AttImp and NpImp (α = 0.75 and 0.72, respectively) subscales and questionable for the MotImp (α = 0.64) subscale.

Functional idiographic assessment template—questionnaire—short form (FIAT-Q-SF)

The short form of the Functional Idiographic Assessment Template—Questionnaire (FIAT-Q-SF) (Darrow et al., 2014) was utilized to assess various aspects associated with the participants’ interpersonal relationships/functioning. Individuals were asked to indicate on a six-point Likert scale (Strongly disagree to Strongly agree) whether or not the statement applies to them. Based on Darrow and colleagues (2014), total overall scores (Total FIAT) were calculated, as well as total scores for each of the following factors:Avoidance of Interpersonal Intimacy (AvdInt), Argumentativeness or Disagreement (ArgDis), Connection and Reciprocity (ConRec), Conflict Aversion (ConAve), Emotional Experience and Expression (EmoExp), and Excessive Expressivity (ExcExp). Higher FIAT-Q-SF subscale or overall scores are indicative of worse interpersonal functioning.

Cronbach’s alpha pertaining to the FIAT-Q-SF indicated good internal consistency for the Total FIAT (α = 0.85), as well as the AvdInt, ArgDis, ConAve and ExcExp subscales (α = 0.88, 0.82, 0.81, 0.82, respectively). Additionally, Cronbach’s alpha indicated acceptable internal consistency for the ConRec subscale (α = 0.72) and questionable internal consistency for the EmoExp subscale (α = 0.64).

Statistical analysis

Analyses were conducted on all data remaining following the application of the exclusion criteria (n= 526) using R version 4.3.0, SigmaPlot version 14.0 (Systat Software, Inc.) and Jamovi version 2.3.15. Proportions tests were utilized to assess (1) the percentage of individuals scoring within poor versus good sleep quality, (2) differences in the percentage of males and females scoring within poor sleep quality and (3) differences in the proportion of those reporting poor sleep quality within each of the BIS-11 scoring categories. Additionally, independent measures t-test (two-tailed) were used to assess (1) sex differences in total/global PSQI, BIS-11 and FIAT-Q-SF scores, (2) sex differences in the subscales/components of each scale utilized in this study (i.e., PSQI, BIS-11, FIAT-Q-SF), and (3) differences in BIS-11 and FIAT-Q-SF total scores, as well as the individual subscales, between participants reporting good vs poor sleep quality (as measured by the PSQI). Pearson correlations were utilized to assess the relationships between the PSQI, BIS-11 and FIAT-Q-SF. Given (1) the various correlations between the variables measured in this study, in addition to (2) previous literature (addressed above) indicating a relationship between sleep and factors associated with both impulsiveness and social relationships, as well as (3) previous research addressing impulsivity as a mediator between variables associated with sociality and well-being (Reichl et al., 2023), mediation analysis using Baron and Kenny’s criteria (Baron and Kenny, 1986; Shrout and Bolger, 2002), as well as 1000 bootstrapping replicates (Shrout and Bolger, 2002), was utilized to investigate the potential relationship between sleep quality (predictor) and interpersonal functioning (outcome), and the potential mediating effect of impulsiveness (mediator) on such a relationship. Differences were considered significant at p < 0.05.

Results

Demographics

The distribution of participants in this survey was 33% male and 67% female, which is relatively representative of the student body at Franciscan University. The data for the demographic questions outlined above are shown across sex in Supplementary Table 1 (See Additional File 1).

Sleep quality (PSQI)

In relation to sleep quality, as measured by the PSQI, average global PSQI scores and average scores for each of the components (as outlined by Buysse et al. (1989)) are shown in Table 1. In this regard, analysis indicated significantly higher global PSQI scores in females relative to males (p < 0.05). Additionally, in relation to the PSQI subscales, females scored significantly higher in both Sleep disturbance (p < 0.05) and Daytime dysfunction due to sleepiness (p < 0.01) relative to males. All other comparisons were not significant (all p > 0.05).

Table 1 Average PSQI, BIS-11 and FIAT-Q-SF scores in overall sample, as well as sex differences

Analysis of sleep quality (PSQI), as defined by Buysse et al. (1989), indicated that a significantly higher proportion [χ2 (1, N = 454) = 97.80, p < 0.001] of participants reported poor (66.5%) relative to good (33.5%) sleep quality. Figure 1 portrays the distribution of participants across global PSQI scores. Additionally, a significantly higher percentage [χ2 (1, N = 454) = 4.78, p < 0.05] of females (70.0%) relative to males (59.2%) reported scores indicating poor sleep quality.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Distribution (percentage) of participants across sleep quality (Global PSQI scores). Coloring indicates poor (red) versus good(gray) sleep quality as defined by Buysse et al. (1989), i.e., scores greater than 5 indicate poor sleep quality

Impulsivity (BIS-11)

Average total BIS-11 scores and the average sum for each subscale (Attentional Impulsiveness, Motor Impulsiveness and Non-planning Impulsiveness) and standard errors of the mean are reported for the overall sample (i.e., sexes combined) and across the sexes in Table 1, as well as statistics (i.e., t-statistics and r-squared values) for the appropriate tests. Average scores are also shown for Total BIS and each subscale across poor and good sleep quality (as measured by the PSQI), as well as appropriate statistics in Table 2. Based on the total score cut-offs addressed in Stanford and colleagues (2009), 26.2% of respondents would be categorized as “highly impulsive” (72 or higher), 62.8% as “within normal limits of impulsiveness” (52-71) and 10.9% as “an individual that is either extremely over-controlled or who has not honestly completed the questionnaire” (< 52).

Table 2 Average scores across BIS-11 and FIAT-Q-SF across poor and good sleep quality (PSQI)

BIS-11 across sex

Analysis of the BIS-11 across the sexes (Table 1; Fig. 2A) indicated no significant difference (p > 0.05) between males and females in the average total BIS-11 scores. Analysis also revealed significantly higher (p < 0.01) average Attentional Impulsiveness scores among females relative to males, while males scored significantly higher (p < 0.05) in Motor Impulsiveness relative to females. In relation to Non-planning Impulsiveness, there was no significant sex difference in average scores (p > 0.05).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Average impulsivity and interpersonal functioning subscale total scores across sex and sleep quality category. A BIS-11 (impulsiveness) subscale totals across sex; B BIS-11 subscale totals across PSQI category (poor vs good sleep quality); C FIAT-Q-SF (interpersonal functioning) subscale totals across sex; D FIAT-Q-SF subscale totals across PSQI category. Data shown as mean ± standard error of the mean. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

BIS-11 and sleep quality

In relation to sleep quality and impulsivity, as measured by the PSQI and BIS-11, respectively, of the individuals scoring within the category of “highly impulsive”, 70.5% reported poor sleep quality. Additionally, of those scoring “within normal limits of impulsiveness”, 55.7% reported poor sleep quality, while 43.6% of those scoring in the lowest category of impulsiveness (< 52) reported poor sleep quality. Proportions test indicated a significantly higher percentage [χ2 (2, N = 503) = 13.76, p < 0.01] of poor sleep quality among those reporting high impulsivity (score 72 or higher) relative to both other groups (score between 52–71: p < 0.05; score < 52: p < 0.01).

Additionally, in relation to impulsivity across sleep quality (Table 2; Fig. 2B), analysis indicated that those reporting poor sleep quality indicated significantly higher average BIS-11 total scores (p < 0.001) than those reporting good sleep quality.

Analysis also revealed significantly higher average Attentional Impulsiveness (p < 0.001) and Non-planning Impulsiveness (p < 0.05) scores, but not Motor Impulsiveness scores (p > 0.05), in participants reporting poor relative to good sleep quality.

Interpersonal functioning (FIAT-Q-SF)

Average total FIAT-Q-SF scores and the average sum for each subscale (Avoidance of Interpersonal Intimacy, Argumentativeness or Disagreement, Connection and Reciprocity, Conflict Aversion, Emotional Experience and Expression, Excessive Expressivity) and standard errors of the mean are reported for the overall sample (i.e., sexes combined) and across the sexes in Table 1, as well as the appropriate statistics (i.e., t-statistics and r-squared values). Average scores are also shown for Total FIAT and each subscale across poor and good sleep quality (as measured by the PSQI), in addition to the appropriate statistics in Table 2.

FIAT-Q-SF across sex

In relation to the FIAT-Q-SF across the sexes (Table 1; Fig. 2C), analysis of the total FIAT-Q-SF scores indicated no significant difference between males and females (p > 0.05). Additional analysis indicated various sex differences across the FIAT-Q-SF subscales. Specifically, males reported significantly higher scores relative to females in Argumentativeness or Disagreement and Connection and Reciprocity (both p < 0.01), while females reported significantly higher Conflict Aversion scores (p < 0.001). All other comparisons were not significant (all p > 0.05).

FIAT-Q-SF and sleep quality

Analysis pertaining to sleep quality (PSQI) and interpersonal functioning (FIAT-Q-SF) (Table 2; Fig. 2D) indicated significantly higher (p < 0.001) Total FIAT scores in those reporting poor relative to good sleep quality. Additionally, analysis indicated significantly higher average Avoidance of Interpersonal Intimacy, Emotional Experience and Expression and Excessive Expressivity scores (all p < 0.01) in those reporting poor relative to good sleep quality.

Correlation of PSQI, BIS-11 and FIAT-Q-SF

Correlation analysis indicated various significant positive relationships between the variables measured pertaining to sleep quality (Global PSQI), impulsivity (BIS-11; Total BIS, Attentional Impulsiveness, Motor Impulsiveness, Non-planning Impulsiveness), and interpersonal functioning (FIAT-Q-SF; Total FIAT, Avoidance of Interpersonal Intimacy, Argumentativeness or Disagreement, Connection and Reciprocity, Conflict Aversion, Emotional Experience and Expression, Excessive Expressivity) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Correlation plot of sleep quality (PSQI), impulsivity (BIS-11) and interpersonal functioning (FIAT-Q-SF). PSQI: Global PSQI Score; BIS-11: BIS, Total BIS; AttImp, Attentional Impulsiveness; MotImp, Motor Impulsiveness; NpImp, Non-planning Impulsiveness; FIAT-Q-SF: FIAT, Total FIAT; AvdInt, Avoidance of Interpersonal Intimacy; ArgDis, Argumentativeness or Disagreement; ConRec, Connection and Reciprocity; ConAve, Conflict Aversion; EmoExp, Emotional Experience and Expression; ExcExp, Excessive Expressivity. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

Mediation analysis of PSQI, BIS-11 and FIAT-Q-SF

Mediation analysis was performed to assess the potential mediating role of impulsiveness (as measured by the BIS-11 subscales; Attentional Impulsiveness, Motor Impulsiveness, Non-planning Impulsiveness; M = mediator in Fig. 4) on the relationship between sleep quality (as measured by the Global PSQI Score; PSQI/IV = independent variable in Fig. 4) and interpersonal functioning (as measured by the Total FIAT-Q-SF Score; FIAT/DV = dependent variable in Fig. 4). Figure 4 shows both the hypothesized conceptual model (Fig. 4A) and the final model (Fig. 4B). Significant mediation pathways, including the indirect relationships, shown in-text (full results shown in Supplementary Table 2 (See Additional File 2)), revealed that the total effect of sleep quality (PSQI) on interpersonal functioning (FIAT-Q-SF) was significant (H1: β = 1.727, t = 6.09, p < 0.001).

Fig. 4
figure 4

The impact of sleep quality (PSQI) on interpersonal functioning (FIAT-Q-SF), mediated through impulsiveness (BIS-11). A Hypothesized mediation model; B Final mediation model. PSQI: Global PSQI Score; BIS-11: AttImp, Attentional Impulsiveness; MotImp, Motor Impulsiveness; NpImp, Non-planning Impulsiveness; FIAT: FIAT-Q-SF Total Score. Data represents estimates/betas—direct (total). IV, independent variable; DV, dependent variable; M, mediator. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

With the inclusion of the mediating variables (BIS-11: Attentional Impulsiveness, Motor Impulsiveness, Non-planning Impulsiveness), the impact of sleep quality (PSQI) on interpersonal functioning (FIAT-Q-SF) was still found significant (β = 0.747, t = 2.48, p < 0.05).

The indirect effect of PSQI on FIAT-Q-SF was found significant through Attentional Impulsiveness (β = 0.987, SE = 0.180, t = 5.47, p < 0.001, 95% CI = 0.678, 1.392). However, the indirect effect of PSQI through both Motor Impulsiveness (β = -0.116, t = -1.76, p = 0.079) and Non-planning Impulsiveness (β = 0.110, t = 1.74, p = 0.083) were not significant.

Discussion

Our study sought to explore the relationship between sleep quality, impulsivity and interpersonal functioning, including the potential mediating role of impulsivity in the dynamic of sleep quality and interpersonal functioning. Our study confirms the significant presence of poor sleep quality and impulsivity among university students. Moreover, corroborating previous reports, our results support the relationship between disordered sleep behaviors, higher levels of impulsivity and lower interpersonal functioning (aan Het Rot et al., 2015; Beattie et al., 2015; Cheng et al., 2021; DeBono et al., 2011; Dorrian et al., 2019; Gillett et al., 2021; Gordon et al., 2021; Parks et al., 2021).

In relation to sex differences, females reported a significantly higher prevalence of poor sleep quality (Global PSQI) relative to males, supporting previous findings (Mong and Cusmano, 2016; Sa et al., 2019). Additionally, while there were no significant sex differences in overall impulsivity (Total BIS) and interpersonal functioning (Total FIAT), differences were observed between males and females in relation to some of the subscales, specifically, Attentional Impulsiveness and Motor Impulsiveness (for the BIS-11), as well as Argumentativeness or Disagreement, Connection and Reciprocity, and Conflict Aversion (for the FIAT-Q-SF).

Pertaining to the impact of sleep quality on impulsivity, individuals reporting poor sleep quality also reported significantly higher levels of overall impulsivity, and higher scores in the specific subscales of Attentional and Non-planning Impulsiveness. Moreover, individuals with poor sleep quality also reported significantly higher total FIAT-Q-SF scores, indicating worse overall interpersonal functioning. Furthermore, within the specific subscales pertaining to interpersonal functioning, those reporting poor sleep quality also reported higher scores in Avoidance of Interpersonal Intimacy, Emotional Experience and Expression and Excessive Expressivity. Correlation analysis revealed significant relationships between sleep quality and various aspects of impulsivity and interpersonal functioning, corroborating the dynamics previously discussed. Additionally, mediation analysis indicated that Attentional Impulsiveness (AttImp), but not Motor (MotImp) or Non-planning Impulsiveness (NpImp), significantly mediated the relationship between sleep quality (PSQI) and interpersonal functioning (FIAT).

Interpersonal functioning, sleep and basic human functioning

In relation to interpersonal functioning, human interactions and relationships are fundamental/essential for normal human behavior (Hawkley and Cacioppo, 2010; Hefner and Eisenberg, 2009). Deprivation from such interactions is known to result in detrimental physiological, and ultimately, psychological effects (Leigh-Hunt et al., 2017; Loades et al., 2020; Sepulveda-Loyola et al., 2020; Taylor et al., 2018). However, the interaction between physiology and complex behaviors, such as those observed in human interactions, is not only bidirectional, but is also mediated/impacted by innate behaviors such as sleep. Through its impact on normal physiological functioning, including but not limited to, molecular and neuronal processes (e.g., Abel et al., 2013; Gaine et al., 2018; Leproult and Van Cauter, 2010), appropriate/inappropriate sleep behaviors (e.g., proper sleep vs insufficient sleep/sleep deprivation/sleep loss, etc.) can significantly impact basic human functioning in relation to learning, memory, decision-making, concentration/attention, and resilience/tolerance to stress (Chatburn et al., 2013; Chattu et al., 2018; Harrison and Horne, 2000; Hudson et al., 2020; Kechter and Leventhal, 2019; Maquet, 2001; Rasch and Born, 2013; Tempesta et al., 2018; Walker and Stickgold, 2005). Consequently, behaviors are impacted at an individual level (e.g., lead to irritability) with subsequent consequences on human relationships/interactions (e.g., Ben Simon et al., 2022; Chattu et al., 2018; Whiting et al., 2023). Our findings appear to support these dynamics through the relationships observed between sleep quality and both impulsivity and interpersonal functioning; specifically, lower sleep quality was associated with higher impulsivity scores and worse interpersonal functioning, thus reinforcing the importance of proper sleep in order to minimize the potential for the dysfunctional behaviors, interactions and relationships addressed above, which can, in the big picture, lead to dysfunction in various life situations including, but not limited to work, school environments, as well as intimate relationships. Additionally, our findings demonstrate that impulsivity (specifically attentional impulsivity) plays a significant role in mediating the relationship between sleep and interpersonal functioning.

While various pharmacological agents can be utilized to buffer the effects of some inappropriate sleep behaviors, such as sleep deprivation, their efficacy is often limited and temporary (e.g., Aggarwal et al., 2011). On the contrary, a more suitable treatment option may be adjustments in behavior such as improving sleep hygiene and routines, which are utilized in behavioral interventions, such as cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia (CBT-I), which have been shown to be more effective in assisting individuals suffering from sleep issues such as insomnia in the long-term (Schlarb et al., 2018; Trauer et al., 2015; Wu et al., 2015). This further highlights the importance of focusing efforts on changing behaviors, rather than simply treating the symptoms associated with poor sleep.

Interpersonal functioning, sleep and executive function

It is important to note that interpersonal functioning is highly influenced by executive functioning (Lewis and Carpendale, 2009; Madjar et al., 2019) through various factors that can be modulated by sleep, such as emotional regulation, decision-making and attention, as indicated above. In fact, at a neurological level, amygdalar (emotion) and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (executive function) dysfunction (which have been shown to relate to sleep deprivation (Goldstein and Walker, 2014; Libedinsky et al., 2011)) have been reported to detrimentally impact emotional control, decision-making and attention (Dorrian et al., 2019; Wolf et al., 2014). This dynamic could potentially be underlying our findings pertaining to the impact of sleep quality on interpersonal functioning, particularly those aspects potentially influenced by emotional regulation (e.g., the Emotional Experience and Expression subscale of the FIAT-Q-SF).

Additionally, attention (which, again, is significantly impacted by executive functioning) is fundamental to all behavior and significantly informs decision-making at all stages of human life (e.g., Huttermann et al., 2018; Rangelov and Mattingley, 2020). The impact of poor sleep quality on executive functioning may also be reflected in the correlation/relationship between sleep quality and impulsivity, most especially in relation to attentional impulsiveness (which represents attention and cognitive instability (Patton et al., 1995)), and to a lesser extent, to motor (motor impulsiveness and perseverance) and non-planning (self-control and cognitive complexity) impulsiveness. Going further into this dynamic, the mediating role of attentional impulsiveness in the relationship between sleep and interpersonal functioning is substantiated by both the aforementioned physiological consequences associated with disordered sleep behaviors, as well as the importance of attention in interpersonal functioning (Capozzi and Ristic, 2018; Pons et al., 2019; Taylor et al., 2016; Wolf et al., 2014). While these findings, based on the specific directionality investigated in our study, further highlight the importance of proper sleep, in order to minimize/avoid negative repercussions associated with both emotion and attention, which have broad implications on human behavior in general, it is necessary to be mindful of the multidirectional relationship between sleep and both emotion and attention (including in association with interpersonal functioning and impulsivity). This multidirectionality is potentially evident in regard to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), where ADHD can lead to disturbed sleep (in addition to emotional dysregulation and reduced attention) or, conversely, disturbed sleep can potentially lead to similar behaviors and symptoms to that of ADHD (Hvolby, 2015; Soler-Gutierrez et al., 2023). Thus, while additional analyses could investigate the alternate relationships between these variables (e.g., the potential mediating role of sleep in the relationship between impulsivity and interpersonal functioning), the implications of our findings and these complex relationships between the variables addressed above highlight the necessity, including at the clinical level, of ensuring that the primary problem is identified and addressed in order to avoid ineffective/inappropriate treatment strategies.

Role of sleep, attention and interpersonal functioning in daily life/functioning

Within the student population

While academics should be the fundamental objective of student life, they do not occur in a vacuum. As per society in general, students are functioning within a social structure that forms the university community and consists of social interactions. As indicated by our findings, students’ interpersonal functioning was negatively impacted by poor sleep quality. Previous literature pertaining to university students has suggested that a lack of social interactions or the presence of negative social interactions have been associated with increased negative psychological consequences (e.g., increased perceived stress, increased proneness to boredom, increased negative emotional wellbeing) (e.g, Dumitrache et al., 2021; Fiori and Consedine, 2013). In addition, other research has indicated the beneficial role of positive social interactions, including indirectly through their influence on other positive behaviors such as physical activity, which has also been shown to be beneficial to well-being (e.g., Kawachi and Berkman, 2001; Reifman and Dunkel-Schetter, 1990; Vankim and Nelson, 2013). While the impact of social interactions on well-being has been recognized extensively in the literature, its relevance became particularly pronounced during COVID-19, a time when many individuals were deprived of in-person social interactions, contributing to a decline in mental health (e.g., Bzdok and Dunbar, 2020; Dumitrache et al., 2021).

Another fundamental aspect pertaining to the university student population that warrants attention is the impact of the students’ lifestyle (e.g., quality of sleep, involvement in sports, etc.) on their academics. Within the student population, a significant amount of literature has indicated the importance of sleep and proper attention in academic functioning and success (e.g., Curcio et al., 2006; Henning et al., 2022; Jalali et al., 2020; Okano et al., 2019). This is of particular concern given the levels of inappropriate sleep, in addition to its consequences on attention, reported both in the current study, as well as other contemporary research (Chua et al., 2017; Massar et al., 2019; Mbous et al., 2022), as addressed above.

Beyond the student population

While our study addressed sleep, impulsivity, and interpersonal functioning specifically in the university student population with its specific characteristics (e.g., studying, learning) and lifestyle (e.g., new social circles, sleep-habit changes, dietary changes), the findings may (taking into consideration certain limitations, e.g., socioeconomic status, life events, etc.) also have broader implications on the general population. In this regard, executive functioning, attention, interpersonal relationships, and the impact of sleep on such behaviors, have the potential to significantly influence numerous common day-to-day human behaviors and tasks (e.g., reading, writing, etc.), including in the context of various work settings (e.g., hands-on labor, operation of equipment, etc.) (Brossoit et al., 2019; Litwiller et al., 2017; Pilcher and Morris, 2020). Thus, as is reflected in the literature, it is not just the student population that suffers the consequences resulting from inappropriate/poor sleep. Ultimately, despite the differences that naturally exist across various social environments (e.g., school, workplace, home), there is a fundamental modus operandi in human physiological and behavioral functioning that is impacted by sleep and therefore, in reality, the implications of our findings are likely generalizable well beyond the student population.

Addressing the issue: the need to focus on the problem

As previously mentioned, pharmacological agents can be utilized to address the negative symptoms experienced as a result of poor/inappropriate sleep. However, these drugs cannot and do not replace the role of sleep as an essential innate physiological behavior. Inappropriate use of pharmacological agents (including in beverages containing caffeine such as, energy drinks, coffee, etc.) may lead to negative repercussions including, but not limited to, an increased propensity for error, including in critical/crucial situations (e.g., surgeon errors, adverse performance and safety outcomes in police officers, etc.) despite the acute relief of the symptoms (Aggarwal et al., 2011; Ogeil et al., 2018). Thus, seeking to relieve symptoms (reactive approach) may be necessary when a problem is already present. However, the long-term goal of any approach must be focused on addressing the root (fundamental underlying cause) of the issue (e.g., Riera-Sampol et al., 2022).

In the context of our study, impulsive behavior was an important factor in the relationships observed. Impulsive behavior underlies negative mental health and a broad spectrum of psychiatric disorders (e.g., Griffin et al., 2018; Smith et al., 2019; Tangney et al., 2004). In contrast, impulse control (i.e., increasing self-control), including its promotion and utilization in therapy (e.g., CBT), has been previously shown to be of benefit in a broad spectrum of behaviors (Smith et al., 2019; Tangney et al., 2004). Thus, encouraging principled behaviors that promote self-discipline/self-control has the potential to reduce negative (e.g., impulsive) and contribute to positive behaviors across a broad spectrum of life circumstances, including, but not limited to, interpersonal relationships, academics and avoidance of destructive behaviors, such as alcohol abuse (Claver et al., 2020; Rogus, 1985; Shi and Qu, 2022; Tangney et al., 2004).

The necessity for principled behaviors in order to achieve success, including in academia, is not a new concept, remembering that university life reflects, in many ways, a microcosm of society. Self-discipline, through its influence on decision-making, ultimately impacts common daily behaviors, such as sleep, diet and general hygiene (Hershner and O'Brien, 2018; Sertillanges, 1946 (1998)), and requires a consistent effort over time, through potential modifications of behavior, to achieve one’s ultimate goal (e.g., improving sleep) (Duckworth et al., 2007; Hagger and Hamilton, 2019; Li and Li, 2021). Such perseverance (sometimes referred to as “grit” in the literature, or “perseverance and passion for long-term goals” (Duckworth et al., 2007) has long term implications, with an ultimate potential outcome of improved overall general and psychological well-being (Duckworth et al., 2007; Lee et al., 2023; Verberg et al., 2019).

Underlying principled behaviors, self-discipline, and perseverance is motivation, which drives personal initiative and, in turn, is dependent on personal autonomy (Cook and Artino, 2016; Patel and Thatcher, 2012). However, given the social nature of humans, and that personal autonomy does not exclude social interaction and community (including, but not limited to, family), but rather includes healthy social relationships while preserving the capacity for self-determination (Ryff, 2014; Vansteenkiste and Ryan, 2013), the cooperation of individuals is necessary in order to maximize the impact of self-discipline. While personal effort is imperative on the side of the individual (e.g., student, employee, etc.) to be self-disciplined (e.g., getting proper sleep, studying, etc.), community support and reinforcement (e.g., in policy-making, in education, by university administration, professors, parents, etc.) is essential. In effect, self-discipline needs to become “everyone’s reality” (Rogus, 1985).

Limitations

The primary limitation of our study is that associations with measures of mental health such as depression, anxiety and stress cannot be made given a direct measure of mental health (e.g., DASS-21) was not included. However, the study did investigate impulsivity, which is an underlying characteristic of various mental health disorders (e.g., Chamorro et al., 2012; Moeller et al., 2001) and reflects a dysfunctional corticolimbic dynamic which is documented extensively clinically and in the scientific literature as underlying psychopathology/psychiatric disorders (e.g., Kovner et al., 2019; Spikman et al., 2000). It is predicted, given previous literature (Alonzo et al., 2021; Zou et al., 2020), that had direct mental health measures been included, individuals reporting poor sleep quality (as measured by the PSQI) would have also reported more negative mental health, similar to what was observed with impulsivity. Related, while psychopathologies that have the potential to impact the variables investigated in this study, such as ADHD (Becker et al., 2018; Sodano et al., 2021) or bipolar disorder (Palagini et al., 2019), were not included as exclusion criteria or covariates, this limitation needs to be considered in the context of the multidirectional relationship of the variables involved in sleep, including its relationship to ADHD as previously discussed in Sect. " Interpersonal Functioning, Sleep and Executive Function".

A consideration pertaining to the interpretation of the results and their implications is the necessity to also consider the limitations (which continue to be debated, most especially in the field of psychology) of any research design and the optimal statistical methods utilized. For example, we utilized t-tests to compare sex differences for the PSQI, BIS-11, FIAT-Q-SF subscales individually. While we chose this test because of the fact that each subscale is a separate entity and we were not comparing between the subscales within a specific scale, the reader needs to consider the potential of an inflated Type-I error rate resulting from this methodology in the interpretation of these results. Additional consideration becomes particularly relevant in relation to the more complex analyses such as mediation analysis. However, while caution is always necessary when implying causation, the reality of the complexity of human behavior and the multidirectional and multicomponent relationships that most investigated human behaviors involve (i.e. the rarity of unidirectional relationships) need to be considered, as well as the consistency and logic of any findings with those of existing research and human experience, which we sought to consider in the discussion of our findings.

Additionally, while our study consisted of a sample size representative of our population, a larger sample size or alternative design (e.g., longitudinal study) could potentially have provided further clarity on certain statistical outcomes that were bordering on significance such as the impulsivity subscales in relation to their potential mediating role. Moreover, while our study included a sample from a single university student population, the findings should be considered in the context of the fundamental consistencies that exist within human behavior across cultures/societies, and the congruity between the findings in our samples and other scientific literature pertaining to university students and the general population. Thus, similar results would be expected in studies investigating other populations, including other university student samples.

An additional point for consideration is that while the scientific literature indicates negative repercussions of COVID-19 on individual’s sleep/sleep quality (e.g., Alimoradi et al., 2021; Alqahtani et al., 2022; Ferreira-Souza et al., 2023; Fiorillo et al., 2020; Mandelkorn et al., 2021; Morin and Carrier, 2021; Son et al., 2020), given the data for this study was collected in 2022, our study cannot provide any direct link to COVID-19 measures, although such an impact cannot be totally dismissed, as also reflected in our previous findings (Emmerton et al., 2024). Additionally, given that this was not a longitudinal study, it is also not possible to make comparisons to the pre-COVID-19 time period. Of note, at the time of the survey administration, the dynamics associated with lockdowns, mask mandates, etc. had been substantially relaxed (Abbasi, 2022; Ballotpedia, 2022).

Conclusions

The levels of poor sleep quality and impulsivity reported both in our study and previous literature are of particular concern, given their potential to negatively impact an individual’s physiological/physical and psychological well-being, which can ultimately influence behaviors, including interpersonal functioning (as indicated by our findings), with the potential to further impact the well-being of the individual. This is particularly important given the continued deterioration in mental health despite the increased resources being made available. Overall, our findings suggest that sleep quality and its relationship with impulsivity, specifically attentional impulsivity, have the potential to impact an individual’s interpersonal functioning.

As appears to be the case in most circumstances in life, in order to achieve overall well-being, when an issue arises, it is ideal that a long-term approach that addresses the root of the problem is considered, rather than solely targeting, in the short-term, the phenotypic “symptoms” resulting from the primary issue. In regard to the current findings, rather than simply seeking to minimize the negative consequences associated with lower interpersonal functioning, it would appear to be more beneficial to maximize preventative constructive behaviors (such as improving sleep quality and minimizing impulsivity, i.e., addressing the root of the issue) that build character/virtue and strengthen the individual, including through self-discipline and perseverance.

Availability of data and materials

The data underlying this article will be shared on reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Abbreviations

PSQI:

Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index

BIS-11:

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Version 11

AttImp:

Attentional Impulsiveness

MotImp:

Motor Impulsiveness

NpImp:

Non-planning Impulsiveness

FIAT-Q-SF:

Functional Idiographic Assessment Template—Questionnaire—Short Form

AvdInt:

Avoidance of Interpersonal Intimacy

ArgDis:

Argumentativeness or Disagreement

ConRec:

Connection and Reciprocity

ConAve:

Conflict Aversion

EmoExp:

Emotional Experience and Expression

ExcExp:

Excessive Expressivity

References

  • aan Het Rot M, Moskowitz DS, Young SN. Impulsive behaviour in interpersonal encounters: associations with quarrelsomeness and agreeableness. Br J Psychol. 2015;106(1):152–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Abbasi E. State by State Face Mask Mandates 2022 Available from: https://leadingage.org/state-state-face-mask-mandates/.

  • Abel T, Havekes R, Saletin JM, Walker MP. Sleep, plasticity and memory from molecules to whole-brain networks. Curr Biol. 2013;23(17):R774–88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Aggarwal R, Mishra A, Crochet P, Sirimanna P, Darzi A. Effect of caffeine and taurine on simulated laparoscopy performed following sleep deprivation. Br J Surg. 2011;98(11):1666–72.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Albers HE. The regulation of social recognition, social communication and aggression: vasopressin in the social behavior neural network. Horm Behav. 2012;61(3):283–92.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Albqoor MA, Shaheen AM. Sleep quality, sleep latency, and sleep duration: a national comparative study of university students in Jordan. Sleep Breath. 2021;25(2):1147–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alimoradi Z, Brostrom A, Tsang HWH, Griffiths MD, Haghayegh S, Ohayon MM, et al. Sleep problems during COVID-19 pandemic and its’ association to psychological distress: a systematic review and meta-analysis. EClinicalMedicine. 2021;36:100916.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Almojali AI, Almalki SA, Alothman AS, Masuadi EM, Alaqeel MK. The prevalence and association of stress with sleep quality among medical students. J Epidemiol Glob Health. 2017;7(3):169–74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Alonzo R, Hussain J, Stranges S, Anderson KK. Interplay between social media use, sleep quality, and mental health in youth: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2021;56:101414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alotaibi AD, Alosaimi FM, Alajlan AA, Bin Abdulrahman KA. The relationship between sleep quality, stress, and academic performance among medical students. J Family Community Med. 2020;27(1):23–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Alqahtani JS, AlRabeeah SM, Aldhahir AM, Siraj R, Aldabayan YS, Alghamdi SM, Alqahtani AS, Alsaif SS, Naser AY, Alwafi H. Sleep Quality, Insomnia, Anxiety, Fatigue, Stress, Memory and Active Coping during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(9);4940. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19094940.

  • Asif N, Iqbal R, Nazir CF. Human immune system during sleep. Am J Clin Exp Immunol. 2017;6(6):92–6.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ballotpedia. State-level mask requirements in response to the coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic, 2020–2022. 2022. Available from: https://ballotpedia.org/State-level_mask_requirements_in_response_to_the_coronavirus_(COVID-19)_pandemic,_2020-2022 .

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51(6):1173–82.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Beattie L, Kyle SD, Espie CA, Biello SM. Social interactions, emotion and sleep: a systematic review and research agenda. Sleep Med Rev. 2015;24:83–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becker SP, Jarrett MA, Luebbe AM, Garner AA, Burns GL, Kofler MJ. Sleep in a large, multi-university sample of college students: sleep problem prevalence, sex differences, and mental health correlates. Sleep Health. 2018;4(2):174–81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Beiter R, Nash R, McCrady M, Rhoades D, Linscomb M, Clarahan M, et al. The prevalence and correlates of depression, anxiety, and stress in a sample of college students. J Affect Disord. 2015;173:90–6.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ben Simon E, Vallat R, Rossi A, Walker MP. Sleep loss leads to the withdrawal of human helping across individuals, groups, and large-scale societies. PLoS Biol. 2022;20(8):e3001733.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Benington JH, Heller HC. Restoration of brain energy metabolism as the function of sleep. Prog Neurobiol. 1995;45(4):347–60.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brossoit RM, Crain TL, Leslie JJ, Hammer LB, Truxillo DM, Bodner TE. The effects of sleep on workplace cognitive failure and safety. J Occup Health Psychol. 2019;24(4):411–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruffaerts R, Mortier P, Kiekens G, Auerbach RP, Cuijpers P, Demyttenaere K, et al. Mental health problems in college freshmen: prevalence and academic functioning. J Affect Disord. 2018;225:97–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bzdok D, Dunbar RIM. The neurobiology of social distance. Trends Cogn Sci. 2020;24(9):717–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Capozzi F, Ristic J. How attention gates social interactions. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2018. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13854.

  • Chamorro J, Bernardi S, Potenza MN, Grant JE, Marsh R, Wang S, et al. Impulsivity in the general population: a national study. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46(8):994–1001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chatburn A, Coussens S, Kohler MJ. Resiliency as a mediator of the impact of sleep on child and adolescent behavior. Nat Sci Sleep. 2013;6:1–9.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chattu VK, Manzar MD, Kumary S, Burman D, Spence DW, Pandi-Perumal SR. The global problem of insufficient sleep and its serious public health implications. Healthcare (Basel). 2018;7(1).

  • Cheng W, Rolls E, Gong W, Du J, Zhang J, Zhang XY, et al. Sleep duration, brain structure, and psychiatric and cognitive problems in children. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26(8):3992–4003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu K, Clark DM, Leigh E. Prospective associations between peer functioning and social anxiety in adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2021;279:650–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chua EC, Fang E, Gooley JJ. Effects of total sleep deprivation on divided attention performance. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(11):e0187098.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Claver F, Martinez-Aranda LM, Conejero M, Gil-Arias A. Motivation, Discipline, and academic performance in physical education: a holistic approach from achievement goal and self-determination theories. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1808.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cook DA, Artino AR Jr. Motivation to learn: an overview of contemporary theories. Med Educ. 2016;50(10):997–1014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Crocker J, Canevello A, Brown AA. Social motivation: costs and benefits of selfishness and otherishness. Annu Rev Psychol. 2017;68:299–325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Curcio G, Ferrara M, De Gennaro L. Sleep loss, learning capacity and academic performance. Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10(5):323–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darrow SM, Callaghan GC, Bonow JT, Follette WC. The Functional Idiographic Assessment Template-Questionnaire (FIAT-Q): initial psychometric properties. J Contextual Behav Sci. 2014;3(2):124–35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dattilo M, Antunes HK, Medeiros A, Monico Neto M, Souza HS, Tufik S, et al. Sleep and muscle recovery: endocrinological and molecular basis for a new and promising hypothesis. Med Hypotheses. 2011;77(2):220–2.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • DeBono A, Shmueli D, Muraven M. Rude and inappropriate: the role of self-control in following social norms. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2011;37(1):136–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dias C, Costa D, Sousa T, Castelhano J, Figueiredo V, Pereira AC, et al. A neuronal theta band signature of error monitoring during integration of facial expression cues. PeerJ. 2022;10:e12627.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dietch JR, Taylor DJ, Sethi K, Kelly K, Bramoweth AD, Roane BM. Psychometric evaluation of the PSQI in U.S. college students. J Clin Sleep Med. 2016;12(8):1121–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dinges DF, Pack F, Williams K, Gillen KA, Powell JW, Ott GE, et al. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4–5 hours per night. Sleep. 1997;20(4):267–77.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dinis J, Braganca M. Quality of sleep and depression in college students: a systematic review. Sleep Sci. 2018;11(4):290–301.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon LJ, Tull MT, Lee AA, Kimbrel NA, Gratz KL. The role of emotion-driven impulse control difficulties in the relation between social anxiety and aggression. J Clin Psychol. 2017;73(6):722–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dorrian J, Centofanti S, Smith A, McDermott KD. Self-regulation and social behavior during sleep deprivation. Prog Brain Res. 2019;246:73–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duckworth AL, Peterson C, Matthews MD, Kelly DR. Grit: perseverance and passion for long-term goals. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007;92(6):1087–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dumitrache L, Stanculescu E, Nae M, Dumbraveanu D, Simion G, Talos AM, et al. Post-lockdown effects on students' mental health in Romania: perceived stress, missing daily social interactions, and boredom proneness. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(16).

  • Emmerton RW, Camilleri C, Sammut S. Continued deterioration in university student mental health: Inevitable decline or skirting around the deeper problems? J Affect Disord Rep. 2024;15:100691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferreira-Souza LF, Julianelli-Pecanha M, Coelho-Oliveira AC, da Silva Bahia CMC, Paineiras-Domingos LL, Reis-Silva A, et al. Impacts of COVID-19 pandemic on sleep quality evaluated by wrist actigraphy: a systematic review. J Clin Med. 2023;12(3).

  • Fetzer Institute / National Institute on Aging Working Group. Multidimensional measurement of religiousness/spirituality for use in health research. Kalamazoo: MI: Fetzer Institute; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiori KL, Consedine NS. Positive and negative social exchanges and mental health across the transition to college. J Soc Pers Relat. 2013;30(7):920–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiorillo A, Sampogna G, Giallonardo V, Del Vecchio V, Luciano M, Albert U, et al. Effects of the lockdown on the mental health of the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic in Italy: results from the COMET collaborative network. Eur Psychiatry. 2020;63(1):e87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gaine ME, Chatterjee S, Abel T. Sleep deprivation and the epigenome. Front Neural Circuits. 2018;12:14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gilboa-Schechtman E, Shachar-Lavie I. More than a face: a unified theoretical perspective on nonverbal social cue processing in social anxiety. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:904.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gillett G, Watson G, Saunders KE, McGowan NM. Sleep and circadian rhythm actigraphy measures, mood instability and impulsivity: a systematic review. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;144:66–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein AN, Walker MP. The role of sleep in emotional brain function. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2014;10:679–708.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon AM, Carrillo B, Barnes CM. Sleep and social relationships in healthy populations: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2021;57: 101428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin SA, Lynam DR, Samuel DB. Dimensional conceptualizations of impulsivity. Personal Disord. 2018;9(4):333–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hagger MS, Hamilton K. Grit and self-discipline as predictors of effort and academic attainment. Br J Educ Psychol. 2019;89(2):324–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harrison Y, Horne JA. The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2000;6(3):236–49.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann Behav Med. 2010;40(2):218–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayashi Y, Rivera EA, Modico JG, Foreman AM, Wirth O. Texting while driving, executive function, and impulsivity in college students. Accid Anal Prev. 2017;102:72–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hayashi Y, Washio Y. Text-message dependency, executive function, and impulsivity in college students: a cluster analysis. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2020;23(11):794–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hefner J, Eisenberg D. Social support and mental health among college students. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2009;79(4):491–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henning C, Summerfeldt LJ, Parker JDA. ADHD and academic success in university students: the important role of impaired attention. J Atten Disord. 2022;26(6):893–901.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hershner S, O’Brien LM. The impact of a randomized sleep education intervention for college students. J Clin Sleep Med. 2018;14(3):337–47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson AN, Van Dongen HPA, Honn KA. Sleep deprivation, vigilant attention, and brain function: a review. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45(1):21–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huttermann S, Helsen WF, Put K, Memmert D. Does visual attention impact on decision making in complex dynamic events? J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2018;40(3):163–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hvolby A. Associations of sleep disturbance with ADHD: implications for treatment. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2015;7(1):1–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jalali R, Khazaei H, Paveh BK, Hayrani Z, Menati L. The effect of sleep quality on students’ academic achievement. Adv Med Educ Pract. 2020;11:497–502.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jentsch JD, Ashenhurst JR, Cervantes MC, Groman SM, James AS, Pennington ZT. Dissecting impulsivity and its relationships to drug addictions. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014;1327:1–26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jones CB, Meier MH, Corbin WE, Chassin L. Adolescent executive cognitive functioning and trait impulsivity as predictors of young-adult risky drinking and alcohol-related problems. Psychol Addict Behav. 2021;35(2):187–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kawachi I, Berkman LF. Social ties and mental health. J Urban Health. 2001;78(3):458–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kechter A, Leventhal AM. Longitudinal Association of Sleep Problems and Distress Tolerance During Adolescence. Behav Med. 2019;45(3):240–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Killgore WD, Balkin TJ, Wesensten NJ. Impaired decision making following 49 h of sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res. 2006;15(1):7–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim TW, Jeong JH, Hong SC. The impact of sleep and circadian disturbance on hormones and metabolism. Int J Endocrinol. 2015;2015: 591729.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kovner R, Oler JA, Kalin NH. Cortico-Limbic Interactions Mediate Adaptive and Maladaptive Responses Relevant to Psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry. 2019;176(12):987–99.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Krakowski M. Violence and serotonin: influence of impulse control, affect regulation, and social functioning. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2003;15(3):294–305.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lee D, Reasoner K, Davidson C, Pennings JS, Lee DH. The relationships between grit, burnout, and demographic characteristics in medical students. Psychol Rep. 2023;126(5):2511–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh-Hunt N, Bagguley D, Bash K, Turner V, Turnbull S, Valtorta N, et al. An overview of systematic reviews on the public health consequences of social isolation and loneliness. Public Health. 2017;152:157–71.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leproult R, Van Cauter E. Role of sleep and sleep loss in hormonal release and metabolism. Endocr Dev. 2010;17:11–21.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis C, Carpendale JI. Introduction: Links between social interaction and executive function. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. 2009;2009(123):1–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li J, Li Y. The role of grit on students' academic success in experiential learning context. Front Psychol. 2021;12.

  • Libedinsky C, Smith DV, Teng CS, Namburi P, Chen VW, Huettel SA, et al. Sleep deprivation alters valuation signals in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Front Behav Neurosci. 2011;5:70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Limone P, Toto GA. Factors that predispose undergraduates to mental issues: a cumulative literature review for future research perspectives. Front Public Health. 2022;10: 831349.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lipson SK, Lattie EG, Eisenberg D. Increased rates of mental health service utilization by U.S. college students: 10-year population-level trends (2007–2017). Psychiatr Serv. 2019;70(1):60–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Litwiller B, Snyder LA, Taylor WD, Steele LM. The relationship between sleep and work: A meta-analysis. J Appl Psychol. 2017;102(4):682–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loades ME, Chatburn E, Higson-Sweeney N, Reynolds S, Shafran R, Brigden A, et al. Rapid systematic review: the impact of social isolation and loneliness on the mental health of children and adolescents in the context of COVID-19. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;59(11):1218-39 e3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Logue SF, Gould TJ. The neural and genetic basis of executive function: attention, cognitive flexibility, and response inhibition. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2014;123:45–54.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lund HG, Reider BD, Whiting AB, Prichard JR. Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. J Adolesc Health. 2010;46(2):124–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Madjar N, Chubarov E, Zalsman G, Weiser M, Shoval G. Social skills, executive functioning and social engagement. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2019;17:100137.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mandelkorn U, Genzer S, Choshen-Hillel S, Reiter J, Meira ECM, Hochner H, et al. Escalation of sleep disturbances amid the COVID-19 pandemic: a cross-sectional international study. J Clin Sleep Med. 2021;17(1):45–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Maquet P. The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science. 2001;294(5544):1048–52.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Massar SAA, Lim J, Sasmita K, Chee MWL. Sleep deprivation increases the costs of attentional effort: performance, preference and pupil size. Neuropsychologia. 2019;123:169–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mbous YPV, Nili M, Mohamed R, Dwibedi N. Psychosocial correlates of insomnia among college students. Prev Chronic Dis. 2022;19:E60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1783–93.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mohammed AR, Kosonogov V, Lyusin D. Is emotion regulation impacted by executive functions? An experimental study. Scand J Psychol. 2022;63(3):182–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mong JA, Cusmano DM. Sex differences in sleep: impact of biological sex and sex steroids. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1688):20150110.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Morin CM, Carrier J. The acute effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on insomnia and psychological symptoms. Sleep Med. 2021;77:346–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Narvaez JC, Magalhaes PV, Trindade EK, Vieira DC, Kauer-Sant’anna M, Gama CS, et al. Childhood trauma, impulsivity, and executive functioning in crack cocaine users. Compr Psychiatry. 2012;53(3):238–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ogeil RP, Barger LK, Lockley SW, O’Brien CS, Sullivan JP, Qadri S, et al. Cross-sectional analysis of sleep-promoting and wake-promoting drug use on health, fatigue-related error, and near-crashes in police officers. BMJ Open. 2018;8(9):e022041.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Okano K, Kaczmarzyk JR, Dave N, Gabrieli JDE, Grossman JC. Sleep quality, duration, and consistency are associated with better academic performance in college students. NPJ Sci Learn. 2019;4:16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Palagini L, Cipollone G, Masci I, Caruso D, Paolilli F, Perugi G, et al. Insomnia symptoms predict emotional dysregulation, impulsivity and suicidality in depressive bipolar II patients with mixed features. Compr Psychiatry. 2019;89:46–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parks KMA, Cardy JEO, Woynaroski TG, Sehl CG, Stevenson RA. Investigating the role of inattention and/or hyperactivity/impulsivity in language and social functioning using a dimensional approach. J Commun Disord. 2021;89:106036.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patel PC, Thatcher SMB. Sticking it out. J Manag. 2012;40(7):1932–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51(6):768–74.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pigeon WR, Pinquart M, Conner K. Meta-analysis of sleep disturbance and suicidal thoughts and behaviors. J Clin Psychiatry. 2012;73(9):e1160–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pilcher JJ, Morris DM. Sleep and organizational behavior: implications for workplace productivity and safety. Front Psychol. 2020;11:45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pons F, Bosch L, Lewkowicz DJ. Twelve-month-old infants’ attention to the eyes of a talking face is associated with communication and social skills. Infant Behav Dev. 2019;54:80–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Potenza MN, de Wit H. Control yourself: alcohol and impulsivity. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010;34(8):1303–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramon-Arbues E, Granada-Lopez JM, Martinez-Abadia B, Echaniz-Serrano E, Anton-Solanas I, Jerue BA. The association between diet and sleep quality among spanish university students. Nutrients. 2022;14(16).

  • Rangelov D, Mattingley JB. Evidence accumulation during perceptual decision-making is sensitive to the dynamics of attentional selection. Neuroimage. 2020;220:117093.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rasch B, Born J. About sleep’s role in memory. Physiol Rev. 2013;93(2):681–766.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reichl D, Heindl B, Distler AL, Steins-Loeber S. Attentional impulsivity accounts for the association of antisociality with craving and mental health problems in incarcerated individuals with substance dependence. Int J Prison Health. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJPH-03-2022-0023.

  • Reifman A, Dunkel-Schetter C. Stress, structural social support, and well-being in university students. J Am Coll Health. 1990;38(6):271–7.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds BW, Basso MR, Miller AK, Whiteside DM, Combs D. Executive function, impulsivity, and risky behaviors in young adults. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(2):212–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riera-Sampol A, Rodas L, Martinez S, Moir HJ, Tauler P. Caffeine intake among undergraduate students: sex differences, sources, motivations, and associations with smoking status and self-reported sleep quality. Nutrients. 2022;14(8).

  • Roeser KA, Somers CL, Mangus LR. Emerging adults’ risk-taking behaviors: personal and social predictors. J Adult Dev. 2018;26(4):256–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogus JF. Promoting Self-discipline: a comprehensive approach. Theory Pract. 1985;24(4):271–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruz M, Tudela P. Emotional conflict in interpersonal interactions. Neuroimage. 2011;54(2):1685–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryff CD. Psychological well-being revisited: advances in the science and practice of eudaimonia. Psychother Psychosom. 2014;83(1):10–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sa J, Samuel T, Chaput J-P, Chung J, Grigsby-Toussaint DS, Lee J. Sex and racial/ethnic differences in sleep quality and its relationship with body weight status among US college students. J Am Coll Health. 2019;68(7):704–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanz-Arigita E, Daviaux Y, Joliot M, Dilharreguy B, Micoulaud-Franchi JA, Bioulac S, et al. Brain reactivity to humorous films is affected by insomnia. Sleep. 2021;44(9).

  • Schlarb AA, Bihlmaier I, Velten-Schurian K, Poets CF, Hautzinger M. Short- and long-term effects of CBT-I in groups for school-age children suffering from chronic insomnia: the kiss-program. Behav Sleep Med. 2018;16(4):380–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scott AJ, Webb TL, Martyn-St James M, Rowse G, Weich S. Improving sleep quality leads to better mental health: A meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Sleep Med Rev. 2021;60:101556.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sepulveda-Loyola W, Rodriguez-Sanchez I, Perez-Rodriguez P, Ganz F, Torralba R, Oliveira DV, et al. Impact of Social Isolation Due to COVID-19 on Health in Older People: Mental and Physical Effects and Recommendations. J Nutr Health Aging. 2020;24(9):938–47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sertillanges AG. The intellectual life: Its spirit, conditions, methods: The Catholic University of America Press; 1946 (1998).

  • Shi Y, Qu S. The effect of cognitive ability on academic achievement: The mediating role of self-discipline and the moderating role of planning. Front Psychol. 2022;13:1014655.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol Methods. 2002;7(4):422–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simoes-Franklin C, Hester R, Shpaner M, Foxe JJ, Garavan H. Executive function and error detection: the effect of motivation on cingulate and ventral striatum activity. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(3):458–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith T, Panfil K, Bailey C, Kirkpatrick K. Cognitive and behavioral training interventions to promote self-control. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2019;45(3):259–79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sodano SM, Tamulonis JP, Fabiano GA, Caserta AM, Hulme KF, Hulme KL, et al. Interpersonal problems of young adults with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Atten Disord. 2021;25(4):562–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soler-Gutierrez AM, Perez-Gonzalez JC, Mayas J. Evidence of emotion dysregulation as a core symptom of adult ADHD: a systematic review. PLoS ONE. 2023;18(1):e0280131.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Son C, Hegde S, Smith A, Wang X, Sasangohar F. Effects of COVID-19 on college students’ mental health in the united states: interview survey study. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22(9):e21279.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Spikman JM, Deelman BG, van Zomeren AH. Executive functioning, attention and frontal lesions in patients with chronic CHI. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22(3):325–38.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stanford MS, Mathias CW, Dougherty DM, Lake SL, Anderson NE, Patton JH. Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: an update and review. Personality Individ Differ. 2009;47(5):385–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swann AC, Lijffijt M, Lane SD, Steinberg JL, Moeller FG. Interactions between bipolar disorder and antisocial personality disorder in trait impulsivity and severity of illness. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;121(6):453–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Pers. 2004;72(2):271–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor CT, Cross K, Amir N. Attentional control moderates the relationship between social anxiety symptoms and attentional disengagement from threatening information. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2016;50:68–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor HO, Taylor RJ, Nguyen AW, Chatters L. Social isolation, depression, and psychological distress among older adults. J Aging Health. 2018;30(2):229–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tempesta D, Socci V, De Gennaro L, Ferrara M. Sleep and emotional processing. Sleep Med Rev. 2018;40:183–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas M, Sing H, Belenky G, Holcomb H, Mayberg H, Dannals R, et al. Neural basis of alertness and cognitive performance impairments during sleepiness. I. Effects of 24 h of sleep deprivation on waking human regional brain activity. J Sleep Res. 2000;9(4):335–52.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Torales J, Kadhum M, Zarate G, Barrios I, Gonzalez I, Farrell SM, et al. Wellbeing and mental health among medical students in Paraguay. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2019;31(7–8):598–602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trauer JM, Qian MY, Doyle JS, Rajaratnam SM, Cunnington D. Cognitive behavioral therapy for chronic insomnia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ann Intern Med. 2015;163(3):191–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai LL, Young HY, Hsieh S, Lee CS. Impairment of error monitoring following sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2005;28(6):707–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Twenge JM, Gentile B, DeWall CN, Ma D, Lacefield K, Schurtz DR. Birth cohort increases in psychopathology among young Americans, 1938–2007: a cross-temporal meta-analysis of the MMPI. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(2):145–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Gaal S, Lamme VA. Unconscious high-level information processing: implication for neurobiological theories of consciousness. Neuroscientist. 2012;18(3):287–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vankim NA, Nelson TF. Vigorous physical activity, mental health, perceived stress, and socializing among college students. Am J Health Promot. 2013;28(1):7–15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vansteenkiste M, Ryan RM. On psychological growth and vulnerability: Basic psychological need satisfaction and need frustration as a unifying principle. J Psychother Integr. 2013;23(3):263–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vedaa O, Erevik EK, Hysing M, Hayley AC, Sivertsen B. Insomnia, sleep duration and academic performance: a national survey of Norwegian college and university students. Sleep Med x. 2019;1:100005.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Verberg F, Helmond P, Otten R, Overbeek G. Mindset and perseverance of adolescents with intellectual disabilities: associations with empowerment, mental health problems, and self-esteem. Res Dev Disabil. 2019;91:103426.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vyazovskiy VV, Delogu A. NREM and REM sleep: complementary roles in recovery after wakefulness. Neuroscientist. 2014;20(3):203–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walker MP, Stickgold R. It’s practice, with sleep, that makes perfect: implications of sleep-dependent learning and plasticity for skill performance. Clin Sports Med. 2005;24(2):301–17, ix.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiting C, Bellaert N, Deveney C, Tseng W-L. Associations between sleep quality and irritability: testing the mediating role of emotion regulation. Pers Individ Dif. 2023;213.

  • Wolf RC, Philippi CL, Motzkin JC, Baskaya MK, Koenigs M. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates visual attention during facial emotion recognition. Brain. 2014;137(Pt 6):1772–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wu JQ, Appleman ER, Salazar RD, Ong JC. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia comorbid with psychiatric and medical conditions: a meta-analysis. JAMA Intern Med. 2015;175(9):1461–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wu JC, Gillin JC, Buchsbaum MS, Chen P, Keator DB, Khosla WuN, et al. Frontal lobe metabolic decreases with sleep deprivation not totally reversed by recovery sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(12):2783–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoo SS, Gujar N, Hu P, Jolesz FA, Walker MP. The human emotional brain without sleep–a prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Curr Biol. 2007;17(20):R877–8.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Young CB, Nusslock R. Positive mood enhances reward-related neural activity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11(6):934–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zohar D, Tzischinsky O, Epstein R, Lavie P. The effects of sleep loss on medical residents’ emotional reactions to work events: a cognitive-energy model. Sleep. 2005;28(1):47–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zou P, Wang X, Sun L, Liu K, Hou G, Yang W, et al. Poorer sleep quality correlated with mental health problems in college students: a longitudinal observational study among 686 males. J Psychosom Res. 2020;136:110177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would also like to acknowledge the assistance provided by Nathan Martin, Paul Gantz, Paul Marlowe, and Rose Prezzia and other members of the Franciscan University psychology club.

Funding

This research was funded by small, private individual, restricted donations made to Franciscan University as a 501(c)3 organization and specifically designated for work currently conducted by SS. Funding for the SurveyMonkey® subscription and gift card drawings was provided to SS by Franciscan University of Steubenville. Neither the donors nor the university had any role in the study design, data collection and analysis, interpretation of results, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SS supervised the study. BJF and SS contributed to the conception, design and conducting of the study. SS, CC, RWE performed the statistical analyses and contributed to the writing of the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the revisions of the manuscript and read and approved the submitted version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen Sammut.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study involved human participants and was reviewed and approved by the Franciscan University of Steubenville Institutional Review Board (IRB: #2022–4). The participants provided informed consent to participate in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Supplementary Table 1. Summary of demographic variables

41606_2024_113_MOESM2_ESM.docx

Additional file 2: Supplementary Table 2. Complete results for mediation pathways between sleep quality (PSQI) and interpersonal functioning (FIAT-Q-SF) with impulsivity (BIS-11: AttImp, MotImp, NpImp) as potential mediators. PSQI = Global PSQI; AttImp = Attentional impulsiveness; MotImp = Motor impulsiveness; NpImp = Non-planning impulsiveness; FIAT = Total FIAT-Q-SF

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Farrell, B.J., Emmerton, R.W., Camilleri, C. et al. Impulsivity mediates the relationship between sleep quality and interpersonal functioning: a cross-sectional study in a sample of university students. Sleep Science Practice 8, 16 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41606-024-00113-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s41606-024-00113-8

Keywords